|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Perfect pitch is a rare talent possessed solely by
the likes of Beethoven, Frank Sinatra and Ella
Fitzgerald, right? Wrong, as Kathryn Brown discovers, it's turning up all
over the place
ELIZABETH WEST MARVIN can always tell which music students
have perfect pitch. They don't necessarily play any differently from her
other students, and they may not always lead the chorus. But they are the
ones who immediately get distracted when the fluorescent lights above their
heads start vibrating with an electrical hum somewhere between B and B-flat.
The uncanny, if sometimes distracting, ability to name a solitary note out
of the blue, without any other notes for reference, is a prized musical
talent-and a scientific mystery. Musicians with perfect pitch-or, as many
researchers prefer to call it absolute pitch-can often play pieces by ear,
and many can transcribe music brilliantly. That's because they perceive the
position of a note in the musical stave-its pitch-as clearly as the fact
that they heard it. Hearing and naming the pitch go hand in hand.
By contrast, most musicians follow not the notes, but the relationships between
them. They may easily recognise two notes as being a certain number of tones
apart, but could name the higher note as an F only if they are told the lower
one is a C, for example. This is relative pitch. Useful, but much less
mysterious.
For centuries, absolute pitch has been thought of as the preserve of the
musical elite. Some estimates suggest that maybe fewer than 1 in 2000 people
possess it. But a growing number of studies, from speech experiments to brain
scans, are now suggesting that a knack for absolute pitch may be far more
common, and more varied, than previously thought. "Absolute pitch is not
an all or nothing feature," says Marvin, a music theorist at the University
of Rochester in New York state. Some researchers even claim that we could
all develop the skill, regardless of our musical talents. And their work
may finally settle a decades-old debate about whether absolute pitch depends
on melodious genes -or early music lessons.
Music psychologist Diana Deutsch at the University of California in San Diego
is the leading voice. Last month at the
Acoustical Society of America [Ref:
Animals7:{Human capabilities of Echo removal}:Animals3 {Snap crackle and
milli-pop}]meeting in Columbus, Ohio, Deutsch reported a study that suggests
we all have the potential to acquire absolute pitch-and that speakers of
tone languages use it every day. A third of the world's population-chiefly
people in Asia and Africa-speak tone languages, in which a word's meaning
can vary depending on the pitch a speaker uses.
Deutsch and her colleagues asked seven native Vietnamese speakers and 15
native Mandarin speakers to read out lists of words on different days.The
chosen words spanned a range of pitches, to force the speakers to raise and
lower their voices considerably. By recording these recited lists and taking
the average pitch for each whole word, the researchers compared the pitches
used by each person to say each word on different days.
Both groups showed strikingly consistent pitch for any given word-often less
than a quarter-tone difference between days. "The similarity," Deutsch says,
"is mind-boggling." It's also, she says, a real example of absolute pitch.
As babies, the speakers learnt to associate certain pitches with meaningful
words-just as a musician labels one tone A and another B-and they demonstrate
this precise use of pitch regardless of whether or not they have had any
musical training, she adds.
Deutsch isn't the only researcher turning up everyday evidence of absolute
pitch. At least three other experiments have found that people can launch
into familiar songs at or very near the correct pitches. Some researchers
have nicknamed this ability "absolute memory", and they say it pops up in
other senses, too. In a 1994 study, for example, Svein Magnussen and Stein
Dyrnes of the University of Oslo in Norway found an absolute memory for visual
images, showing that people could pick out complex black-and-white line designs
they had seen hours or days earlier from a selection of very similar ones.
Given studies like these, the real mystery is why we don't all have absolute
pitch, says cognitive psychologist Daniel Levitin of McGill University in
Montreal. "I don't have to run to a rainbow and find red to tell you that
a tomato is red," Levitin says. "There are 10 basic colours that everyone
can name immediately. Well, there are
12 basic
pitches. If we can label all those colours, why can't we label all those
pitches?" Levitin suspects he knows the answer. Absolute pitch, he says,
is really a two-step process: pitch memory and pitch labelling. It's not
that people with absolute pitch are genetically endowed with a keener sense
of pitch perception, Levitin says-after all, many of us can recall a note
nearly perfectly immediately after we hear it. But people with absolute pitch
automatically connect the memory of a pitch with a label. Some even describe
different pitches as having distinct "colours" or "characters".
Lacking absolute pitch, most of us can't make that connection-labelling a
note as "D", for example. But do the connections and labels get hammered
in during music lessons, or are some babies just born with a flair for
identifying pitch? That's a hard question to answer, since musical parents
often pass a passion for music-as well as their genes-on to their children.
Over the past decade, researchers have confirmed that absolute pitch often
runs in families. Nelson Freimer of the University of California in San
Francisco, for example, is just completing a study that he says strongly
suggests the right genes help create this brand of musical genius. Freimer
gave tone tests to people with absolute pitch and to their relatives. He
also tested several hundred other people who had taken early music lessons.
He found that relatives of people with absolute pitch were far more likely
to develop the skill than people who simply had the music lessons. "There
is clearly a familial aggregation of absolute pitch," Freimer says.
Blossoming talent
Freimer says some children are probably genetically predisposed toward absolute
pitch-and this innate inclination blossoms during childhood music lessons.
Indeed, many researchers now point to this harmony of nature and nurture
to explain why musicians with absolute pitch show different levels of the
talent. "The early learning period-from about three to six years of age-is
critical" says Marvin. But lucky genes probably help, she adds. Indeed,
researchers are finding more and more evidence suggesting music lessons are
critical to the development of absolute pitch. In a survey of 2700 students
in American music conservatories and college programmes, New York University
geneticist Peter Gregersen and his colleagues found that a whopping 32 per
cent of the Asian students reported having absolute pitch, compared with
just 7 per cent of non-Asian students. While that might suggest a genetic
tendency towards absolute pitch in the Asian population, Gregersen says that
the type and timing of music lessons probably explains much of the difference.
For one thing, those with absolute pitch started lessons, on average, when
they were five years old, while those without absolute pitch started around
the age of eight. Moreover, adds Gregersen, the type of music lessons favoured
in Asia, and by many of the Asian families in his study, such as the Suzuki
method, often focus on playing by ear and learning the names of musical notes,
while those more commonly used in the US tend to emphasise learning scales
in a relative pitch way. In Japanese preschool music programmes, he says,
children often have to listen to notes played on a piano and hold up a coloured
flag to signal the pitch. "There's a distinct cultural
difference," he says.
If the right genes and music lessons do prompt people to label tones in a
fundamentally different way, then this cognitive difference should show up
in their brains. As indeed it does. In a 1998 study neuroscientist Robert
Zattorre of the Montreal Neurological Institute in Canada ran positron emission
tomography (PET) scans of musicians with and without absolute pitch while
they listened to tones.
When asked to label a tone, the musicians lacking absolute pitch had a flash
of brain activity in the right frontal cortex-an area associated with working
memory and comparing incoming sensory information with memories. By contrast,
the musicians who had absolute pitch could identify tones without accessing
working memory at all. Instead, they showed a spark of brain activity high
in the left frontal cortex-a region related to long-term memory. Zattore
suggests that the absolute pitch users were tapping into a more deeply ingrained
pitch template that they developed during childhood lessons.
A study led by musicologist Laura Bischoff of Shepherd College in West Virginia
also shows that people with the strongest absolute pitch skills can name
notes without working memory. Bischoff and her colleagues gave 32 music
students-half of whom had absolute pitch-a series of tone tests while the
students wore a jumble of scalp electrodes. The researchers were looking
for a working memory marker: the P300, a positively charged waveform that
flashes across the brain 300 milliseconds after a surprising stimulus. The
P300 is thought to indicate a comparison of incoming sensory stimuli-such
as a new tone-with memorised information, in this case a musical scale.
During one test, the students listened to a typical scale, trying to guess
whether the note being played fitted within the scale. At first, the notes
would build predictably, neatly forming a scale in the key of C. But then
a tone would jump out of scale, falling unexpectedly flat or sharp. Scrambling
to name that errant tone, the students without absolute pitch showed a P300
surge, as expected, while most of the students that had absolute pitch did
not.
But the experiment also showed how varied a talent absolute pitch can be.
Four of Bischoff's absolute pitch students showed brain wave patterns more
like those in the control group. Further tests revealed that these absolute
pitch students alternated between absolute and relative depending
on the task at hand.
The lesson,Bischoff says, is that absolute pitch is not a one-fits-all talent.
Some people have an acute sense of absolute pitch, while others show just
a hint of the skill. And some absolute pitch possessors use it only occasionally,
flipping back to relative pitch when that skill is more useful.
A bit of both
That doesn't surprise Philip Chang, a music theory graduate student at Rochester.
While he's had absolute pitch since he was a child, Chang has also had training
that hones relative pitch skills- practising scales, recognising intervals
and so on. "I just use what's helpful," he says.
But can anyone develop absolute pitch? Bischoff thinks so. "Our studies tie
right in with the idea that we all have this latent absolute pitch ability,
but we can't get fully bloomed absolute pitch without early childhood training,"
says Bischoff.
But some scientists are more cautious. After all, if everyone remembered
pitches, but just couldn't label them, we'd immediately know if something
was played in an unusual key, or if two songs started on the same note, says
psychologist Andrea Halpern of Bucknell University in Lewisburg, Pennsylvania.
These feats, she says, are reserved for people with absolute pitch.
Similarly, linguists are wary of the idea that consistently speaking in a
given pitch range somehow reflects absolute pitch. People naturally settle
into a comfortable range while talking, says Rebecca Herman, a linguist at
Indiana University. Deutsch counters that this "comfort zone" argument can't
explain the exceedingly small differences in pitch among her speakers.
Indeed, Deutsch predicts that further studies will reveal absolute pitch-in
its imperfect, latent form-inside all of us. The Western emphasis on relative
pitch simply obscures it, she contends. "It's very likely that scientists
will end up concluding that we're all born with the potential to acquire
very fine-grained absolute pitch. It's really just a matter of life getting
in the way."
Musical roots may lie in human voice
13:28 06 August 03 NewScientist.com news service
Key universal features in world music may have
their roots in the ever-present sound of the human voice during the course
of evolution, suggests a new study. The analysis of thousands of recorded
speech samples found peaks in acoustic energy that precisely mirror the distances
between important notes in the twelve-tone scale, the system that forms the
foundation of almost all music. "The mysteries of music have a biologically
principled explanation," says Dale Purves, at Duke University, North Carolina,
and lead author of the study. "A reasonable speculation is that we hear these
tonal relationships because they are involved in our interpretation of each
other's speech." As a slide whistle shows, it is possible to change seamlessly
the pitch of a sound from low to high and back again. But for making music,
human cultures have sliced the pitch dimension into twelve distinct tones.
This twelve-tone
"chromatic scale"
can be heard by starting at any piano key and then playing the next dozen
white and black keys in succession. On the thirteenth note, the scale begins
again, one octave higher.
Pythagoras's theorem
Different musical traditions have characteristic sound because many cultures
have devised scales from a subset of the full chromatic scale, with different
distances, or "intervals," between the tones. Chinese music is based on five-tone
scales, while scales common in Western music have seven tones. But all cultures
favour certain intervals from the chromatic scale, and listeners judge these
same intervals to create the most harmonious combinations of two tones.
Pythagoras proposed that such preferences could
be predicted from mathematical relationships
between tones, but these approaches have yet to provide a complete
explanation. The Duke researchers randomly extracted over 100,000 speech
samples, each 0.1 second long, from recordings of thousands of English sentences.
Acoustic analysis of the combined samples revealed 10 frequency peaks that
match the most significant intervals used in musical scales worldwide.
Mandarin and Farsi
Moreover, the relative heights of the peaks backed numerous studies in which
listeners ranked the harmoniousness of intervals. Speech in other languages
- Mandarin, Farsi, and Tamil - also displayed the same pattern. The frequency
peaks are caused when a sound wave from the vocal cords is shaped by resonances
of the throat and oral cavity. The researchers say that, aside from animal
calls, speech emanating from oscillations of the human vocal cords is virtually
the only natural sound that we hear as tones. This fact, combined with the
new finding that preferred musical intervals are better predicted by the
acoustic quirks of the human vocal tract than by mathematics, leads the
scientists to argue that the structure of music is rooted in our long exposure
to the human voice over evolutionary time. Journal reference: Journal of
Neuroscience (vol 23, p 7160)
Peter Farley
|
Perfect pitch
|
Question In musical circles it is often claimed
that the key or pitch of a piece can have a profound bearing on the mood
conveyed. For example, if played in the key of E major, the music may be
considered bright and powerful but in F major, peaceful or contemplative.
This is surprising, given that all keys contain similar intervals when played
in equal temperament. Few people have perfect pitch and the frequency shift
in the above example is only one semitone, just under 6 per cent. It seems
unlikely therefore that such an effect could be consistently experienced
by a wide range of listeners. Have any tests been conducted, perhaps by simply
playing recordings at different speeds and, if confirmed, what might be the
physiological basis?
John Allsop , Rayleigh Essex
Answers
Keys used to have distinctive characters, although these died out during
the last century when the equal temperament tuning system became standard.
A key's character is largely determined by the "major third", a musical interval
between the pitch of two notes which appears in all common chords. Ideally,
the frequencies should be in the exact ratio of 4:5. If they are, the pitches
blend perfectly to produce a warm, mellow sound, and the interval is called
a "natural third". A major third that is larger (sharper) than this, gives
the key a bright character. If the major third is too small (flat), the key
sounds dull. It is mathematically impossible to tune the major thirds of
all keys to this ideal natural third--on average, major thirds have to be
slightly sharp, as is the case in equal temperament. So in the past, only
frequently used keys were tuned with natural thirds. As a consequence, the
less common keys ended up with distinctly sharp major thirds, and rarely
used keys were badly out of tune. Regardless of the tuning system used, the
general result is the same. The commonest keys of C, F and G major have natural
thirds and so sound mellifluous; Bb, D, Eb and A are neutral; E and Ab are
bright to jangly and the remote keys of B, C# and F# make humans wince and
dogs howl. This fits the questioner's descriptions of E and F major. Because
of this effect, in the past composers allegedly chose keys to suit the character
of the music. With equal temperament, however, all keys now have identical,
neutral intervals, so this effect is lost.
Ben Finn , Cambridge
Until the 19th century, there really were differences between one key and
another. Only in modern times has equal temperament given every musical key
exactly the same intervals as any other. So in the past there may have been
some objective basis for claiming different keys felt different. Nowadays
this claim is purely subjective, although it may be influenced by a person's
memories of other pieces of music in the same key, or by symbolic factors
that are connected to factors other than sound--Mozart used Eb major as the
masonic key, because it had three flats and three was the Freemasons' mystical
number.
Peter Jeffrey , Music Department Princeton University
Even though most of us do not have perfect pitch, we can detect small differences
in pitch when notes are played together or one after the other. The pitch
of the stringed instruments can be varied infinitely, and these instruments
often tend to play more closely to a natural scale than to the equal tempered
scale that other instruments use. As the difference between the scales is
less than 1 per cent, this effect does not make the instrument sound out
of tune, but it affects the tonal quality of the whole orchestra when instruments
of different kinds play together. To a certain extent, wind instrumentalists
can also vary their pitch by varying the way they blow. However, the strings
have some immutable notes: for example, the open strings on a violin are
tuned to G, D, A and E. This means that some instruments play slightly out
of tune, and the effect varies from key to key.
Terry Moore
"Absolute" pitch is irrelevant to the "colour" of various tonalities. If
the only instrument available were a piano precisely tuned to 12 equally
spaced semitones there would be little point writing music other than in
C major because it would sound the same in any other key, only higher or
lower. A wind instrument such as the recorder, on the other hand, uses a
variety of "forked fingerings" for most of the semitones and is, in fact,
a different instrument when played in C major or C# minor, for example. This
is because the hand has too few fingers, and the recorder too few holes,
to render all the semitones correctly and also because the tone (harmonics)
of the instrument changes audibly from one forked fingering to another. It
is to enhance these effects that good composers have exploited the built-in
inequalities of tuning and tone of the various instruments.
J Azad , Calgary Canada
John Allsop is asking the wrong questions. By suggesting that few listeners
would experience any change of musical colour as a result of a semitone pitch
change, he is assuming that humans listen to (and play) music algorithmically,
like a computer processing an incoming waveform. This is not the case. In
fact, there are a great many factors which influence our perception of musical
colour. If we consider the piano, the "shape" of a chord, its distribution
of black and white notes and the position of the chord up or down the keyboard
can have a considerable effect on the way it sounds. Some composers take
advantage of this to achieve a particular tonal colour. Stringed instruments
have fixed points in their range--when the strings are open. Certain keys
are very much easier to play in than others, and resonance of the open strings
plays a part in colouring keys. A further vital factor is the listener's
cultural background. If we learn we are about to hear a work in C minor,
we think of Beethoven's Third Piano Concerto or the Mozart Piano Concerto
in that key--all are dark, tragic works. We presuppose that anything else
in that key will be similar.
David Stater
As a young musician and composer I'm surprised never to have heard of this
saying, and it's certainly not borne out that well by my experience. As part
of music A-level I studied Bach's Brandenburg Concerto No 5 which is bright,
happy and in F Major! I suspect that this saying refers to the fact that
before the system of "equal temperament" was brought in, there was a slight
difference between, for example, C sharp and D flat. Hence the scale in a
"sharp" key (like E major) was slightly different to that of flat keys (like
F major), and might account for the mood difference. With equal temperament
(where C sharp and D flat are the same) this shouldn't apply, but composers
would probably keep writing the same style in the same key, hence the
relationship appears to stay.
Joe Wakeling , Monmouth
Until the early nineteenth century the use of different temperaments for
tuning instruments meant that music did sound different when transposed to
other keys, and composers consciously used these effects. Music was considered
a science, whose physical and mathematical attributes were exploited from
Pythagoras to Bach. More dubiously, theorists from the Middle Ages to the
eighteenth century tried to equate musical intervals with signs from the
zodiac, theories of the humours (fire, earth, water and air) the music of
the celestial spheres, and so on. Even Isaac Newton dabbled in some of this.
Music played on an electronic instrument in equal temperament should sound
satisfactory in any key provided you only want to hear the tune and ignore
any resonance that comes with a different pitch. If you are used to singing
along to a familiar melody you will detect a large change in pitch because
your voice is used subconsciously as a personal tuning fork. A piano may
sound different in different keys because the hammers are more worn down
or unevenly adjusted on different notes. But essentially in equal temperament
there is no difference of mood between one key and another. Setting aside
the matter of perfect pitch, musicians develop an awareness of pitch through
experience and repetition. The brain analyses a spectrum of sound and compares
it with a "catalogue" held in the memory. A string quartet in B flat played
on instruments tuned a few cents above or below standard concert pitch would
not sound noticeably different to most listeners, so long as the two pitches
were not heard closely following one after the other. Tuning the instruments
up or down as much as a semitone one will radically alter their natural string
tension and resonance, changing the whole character of the sound. Similarly,
players transposing to a natural or B natural alter the use of open strings
and choice of fingering, thereby affecting the resonance in a different way.
Ian Gammie , St Albans Hertfordshire
Being myself a composer of symphonic music, I have had a particular interest
in this very matter. For many years I assumed that it was a complete fallacy
that particular keys had particular absolute qualities. After all, concert
pitch these days is, I understand, something like a whole tone higher than
in Mozart's day, so his celebrated G Minor symphony,
for example, is nowadays played in something like the A minor of Mozart's
day-yet we accept today's performance as a reasonable representation of Mozart's
intentions. In fact the issue is considerably more complex than this. There
are two main factors to be borne in mind. One is the actual pitch of the
music, and the other is the sound-the timbre-of each instrument at a particular
pitch. Let's briefly look at these. Absolute pitch: some of your readers
may be surprised to learn that there is a strong evidence that many, if not
most, people do have intrinsic perfect pitch. Through lack of training they
haven't developed the ability to recognise pitches at a conscious level by
name, but with appropriate training they could develop something of this
ability. For at least most of us, particular absolute pitches do have their
specific resonances in the mind, and therefore it is to be expected that
even a semitone transposition of a musical work would cause at least a small
difference in the effect of the work. Timbre: the
sound and emotional effect of a musical scale is determined not only by the
particular sequence of musical intervals at specific pitches, but also by
the changes of timbre as the instrument moves from pitch to pitch. Also,
string players have to change from one string to another at different points,
and wind players similarly have to change between fundamental and harmonic
or between different harmonics at particular points in their range. Here's
a little experiment to try if you have the resources. Take a real orchestra
and take a recording of them playing Mozart's G Minor Symphony tuned a tone
lower than normal to approximate to the likely original concert pitch for
that work. Also take a recording of them playing a version of the work transposed
down a whole tone-that is, in the root key in F minor. Compare the recordings.
You'd find that the two similarly-pitched performances had a different sound
and at least some difference of emotional quality.
Philip Goddard , Exeter Devon
There may be some recent tests, but for a thorough examination of this topic
see "The Science of Music" by Sir James Jeans, first published in 1937 but
still available as a Dover paperback. Jean refutes the notion that, under
equal temperament, there can be any emotional difference between keys, although
he accepts that many musicians believe that there is. He puts it down to
subjective imagination. He cites the old Lydian and Aeolian modes, which
are equivalent to our major and minor scales. The Lydian (major) mode was
associated with sorrow in Plato's time, and in the early church it was frowned
upon as being too sensual-neither association matches our current perception.
The issue is complicated by the repertoire. Before equal temperament became
accepted, keyboards used mean tone tuning, and compositions were restricted
to a few keys centred on C major, which was perfectly in tune. Remote keys,
such as C sharp, were completely unplayable. This clearly had an influence
on the way composers wrote music, even after equal temperament had ironed
out the differences between keys. C major and its relatives for straightforward
music, remote keys for introverted and melancholy music, such as Schubert's
G flat Impromptu. Thus, we have an association which remains with us, even
today. It has also been suggested that the relative length and width of the
keys on a piano makes a difference to the force with which black and white
notes can be played: C major is all white, and can be played with great noisy
crashes that are very difficult on black notes. On stringed instruments,
the open keys (G, D, A and E for a violin) have similar characteristics to
C on a piano, given that it is easier to play a scale which includes several
open strings than a scale where every note has to be fingered.
Christopher Lambton , Edinburgh
The Vibrating String
The basic principle underlying all western musical tuning systems has been
ascribed to Pythagoras, who lived in the sixth century BC. There is no hard
evidence for this, but whatever its precise origin, the system itself is
the result of a brilliant piece of inductive logic, which impresses by its
simplicity and "correctness". The system derives from the vibrations of a
taut string, which inherently is capable of vibrating in a number of different
ways. The simplest is where half a wavelength fills the string, but in principle
the string can sustain any vibration involving a whole number of half waves.
The possible combinations lie in the ratios: 1:2:3:4:5:6:7:8:9:10: ............
etc. This progression simply records the number of half wavelengths in the
string, but it is a simple matter to see that, for any given length and tension,
it also represents the ratios of vibrational frequency of the string in each
of the successive modes of vibration. This progression of frequencies is
called the harmonic series.
The Discovery of Harmony
What Pythagoras is said to have discovered is that certain components of
the harmonic series sound pleasant, or harmonious, when sounded together.
This is true for any combination of the first six harmonics in the series:
1:2:3:4:5:6. The first, and obvious, implication of this progression is the
repeated occurrence of a basic harmonious interval of 1:2, as shown in the
ratios 1:2, 2:4 and 3:6. This interval is now called the "octave". . The
next component, the seventh harmonic, is the first which sounds discordant
in relation to any of the others. This will be familiar to players of the
bugle as the horribly sharp high A, which is notoriously easy to hit by accident.
If we seek harmony, we do best to stop at six.
Creation of a Scale
To create a more finely-divided musical scale from this series of numbers
involved two distinct logical stages. The first of these was to recognise
that the ratio of 1:2 is equivalent to both 2:4 and 3:6. By eliminating these
duplicated ratios, the harmonious series was simplified to its inherent ratios:
2:4:5:6. Mathematically, this is simply the removal of redundant information;
by very simple processes of integer arithmetic, any of the combinations in
1:2:3:4:5:6 can be obtained from 2:4:5:6. The second logical stage was to
use this simplified series of ratios to subdivide the large interval of 1:2
into smaller intervals. In principle, this subdivision can be taken to any
desired degree depending on the need for simplicity or expressiveness in
the scale. A scale of only four notes is possible, but the results are rather
unsubtle, as witness the rather obvious melodies written for unvalved brass
instruments such as the bugle. By contrast, some oriental tuning systems
have a large number of closely-spaced notes, allowing many more possible
combinations and a much greater degree of expressiveness. In western music,
the basic division has, since the very earliest times, been into a scale
consisting of eight notes. After eight notes the scale repeats itself, hence
the derivation of the word octave.
The Diatonic Scale
The subdivision between the notes of the western scale takes the form shown
below: Succeeding notes are in ratios of 8:9, 9:10 or 15:16. Although not
equal, the ratios of 8:9 and 9:10 are called tones, while the closer ratio
of 15:16 is called a semitone. Because of the two types of interval, the
term diatonic scale is used. The intervals run: tone: tone: semitone: tone:
tone: tone: semitone.
The Sense of Key
Because of the asymmetric structure of the scale, it is very easy to spot,
from hearing just a few notes, precisely where in the scale they lie. This
gives a very powerful identity to the scale; its organisation points very
clearly to the home note, or tonic, which in the scale shown above is C.
We intuitively expect a musical statement to be resolved by a return to the
tonic note; if this does not happen, an open-ended and incomplete impression
is created.
Modal Scales
Medieval music often used modal scales, in which the need for sharps or flats
was ignored when starting a scale on a note other than C. The basic diatonic
pattern of tones and semitones is replaced by a different pattern for each
mode, giving rise in some cases to eerie and expressive music. The modal
scales can be heard easily by playing only the white notes on a piano, but
starting and ending on a note other than C.
The Chromatic Scale
An obvious extension of the diatonic scale is to split the whole tones into
semitones, so that, for example, the note C, increased in the ratio 16:15,
becomes an intermediate note called C sharp (C#). The notes E and B obviously
cannot have sharps, and so an enlarged scale of just 12 notes is produced.
Because some of the harmonies in the enlarged scale are strange, and technically
discordant, the term Chromatic Scale was adopted to describe the sometimes
highly-coloured sound of the semitone scale. By an equivalent process, any
note of the scale, except for C and F, can be flattened in the ratio 15:16
to produce its equivalent flat. (Symbol: b). A crucial point to note is that
the frequency of a flat note is never the same as that of the equivalent
sharp. Thus, for example, Eb and D# are different notes which sound at different
pitches.
The Use of Different Keys
The main historical reason for the development of the chromatic scale is
connected with a desire to change the pitch at which a particular piece of
music is heard. The most obvious reason for such a key change is to match
an instrument to the most comfortable range of a human voice, but, by the
middle ages, key transitions were in widespread use, in their own right,
as a means of introducing contrast between different passages of music. In
order to retain the fundamental tone to semitone ordering of the diatonic
scale, certain notes must be replaced by their sharp or flat equivalents
if the scale is to start and end on any note other than C. This gives rise
to the key signature, the pattern of sharp or flat symbols which appears
at the opening of a score.
The Sound Quality of Different Keys
Because the interval of a tone may be either 8:9 or 9:10, the internal frequency
relationships within a scale will depend on the key which is in use. For
an example, the key of C starts with a ratio of 8:9, whereas the key of D
starts with a ratio of 9:10. Worked out fully for these two keys we find
the following frequency ratios: The internal differences between these two
keys are fairly subtle, but in every key there is a different pattern, and
for some, the differences are so great that the overall effect can be markedly
discordant. Composers have always had views on the subjective emotional quality
of different keys. Beethoven, for example, regarded the key of Eb as heroic
in sound, and used it for both the Emperor Concerto and the Eroica Symphony,
and also, in its equivalent minor mode, for his Fifth Symphony. As we shall
see shortly, by the time of Beethoven, such views could not be sustained
in terms of internal frequency intervals within individual keys, because
the issue of tuning had reached such a critical state that an entirely new
system of tuning had come into being.
The Equal Temperament Scale
Well before the start of the eighteenth century, the technical quality of
musical instruments, and the virtuosity of their players had created a major
crisis in the technicalities of tuning. At a fundamental level, composers
and performers wanted to use harmonic effects and key progressions which,
quite simply, sounded unacceptably discordant when used together. This was
really the end for the Pythagorean tuning system, which could only provide
perfect tuning for one key at a time. If a performer wished to change key,
his instrument would, in theory, have to be retuned. Various solutions to
this problem were sought, all involving the basic principle of compromise
tuning; sharing out the errors in tuning throughout the whole chromatic scale
in such a way that the widest use could be made of an instrument's capabilities.
Many laudable attempts were made to retain the most fundamental harmonic
progression of the major triad, the mathematically-precise 4:5:6 relationship
of the notes C:E:G, but all these are now history; since the time of Bach,
the principle of Equal Temperament has been in almost continuous and universal
use. The equal temperament scale does the most radical and mathematically-precise
job possible of dividing up the tuning errors between the notes. The mathematical
principle is that the octave, with a frequency range of 1:2, is divided into
twelve precisely equal geometric steps, to give the twelve semitones of the
diatonic scale. Each note of the scale can be calculated from the previous
note by multiplying its frequency by 21/12 (=1.0595).
Plus and Minus
The overall consequences of the equal temperament scale are a mixed bag.
On the positive side, an instrument may be played in any key with no change
in tonal relationships, and flats and sharps become truly equivalent (eg
Ab = G#). On the negative side, every single note in the scale (except for
the octaves) is out of tune to some extent, with the result that the
intrinsically harmonious ratios of 4:5:6 are never heard exactly in modern
performances. There are a few recorded examples of performances on keyboard
instruments with Pythagorean tuning, with results which are said by reviewers
to be a "revelation".
Additional Notes
Chromatic Scales. The table shows the tuning for each semitone in the scale
of C, based on the modern standard pitch with A fixed at 440 Hz. Frequencies
are in Hertz, Py = Pythagorean Tuning, Eq = Equal Temperament.
Nick Pillans , London
|
Mother's darlings
When Dad's a stunner his chicks get special treatment |
FEMALE zebra finches give chicks fathered by their favourite
mates a boost when they are still in the egg. The finding could be a headache
for biologists, as this kind of trick makes it difficult to tell whether
the father's genes or the mother's extra help allows offspring to thrive.
Several studies of birds have shown that females tend to go for highly ornamented
males, and that when they do, their offspring have a better chance of survival.
It is generally thought that this is because these males have superior genes.
|
|
However, testing this has been complicated by
the fact that some mothers seem to invest more in raising chicks fathered
by good males. Some experiments have controlled for this effect but evolutionary
biologist Diego Gil of the University of Paris in Nanterre suspected that
there may be an even bigger problem.
Gil and his colleagues at the University of St Andrews had a hunch that females
also give chicks from favoured mates a helping hand by depositing more
testosterone in the egg. Chicks in eggs with high levels of testosterone
are usually the first to hatch, beg more for food and grow faster than the
others.
The researchers randomly attached either red or green bands to the legs of
a number of male zebra finches (Taeniopygia
guttata). Females find the lure of the red bands even more attractive
than the male's traditional ornament its red beak. They then allowed six
females to mate with the red males, and six to mate with the greens. The
clutches of eggs produced by these liaisons were taken away for hormone analysis.
The two groups of females were then swapped around and allowed to mate with
males of the other colour.
The females deposited significantly more testosterone in all the eggs from
the preferred red-banded males (Science, vol 286, p 126). The researchers
aren't sure how this happens, but it could be that the females' general levels
of arousal increased their own hormone levels, which in turn affected the
eggs.
This doesn't mean that there is no connection between ornaments and superior
genes -but it does make testing the idea a lot harder "This is a remarkable
study-it raises lots of new and exciting questions," says bird behaviour
expert Tim Birkhead of the University of Sheffield. "That clearly you have
to take these maternal effects into account is an important message for all
those people looking at good genes." Matt Walker [New
Scientist 9/10/1999] |
The Call of the Wild
|
Humans may have to search high and
low for traces of absolute pitch, but other animals flaunt the talent.
Researchers have found that bats,
wolves, gerbils and birds all sometimes
use forms of absolute pitch to spot possible mates-or meats-amid nature's
cacophony.
Songbirds, in particular, put humans to shame, in a 1998 Study, psychologist
Ron Weisman at Queen's University in Ontario, Canada, pitted 10 male
zebra finches against 10 accomplished
musicians. The birds had to decide whether to fly to a feeder that opened
only when tones within four narrow frequency ranges were played, while the
humans pushed a button to indicate whether a pitch was in one of the designated
ranges-and won prize money for correct answers.
Towards the end of the experiment, Weisman says, the feathered participants
identified pitch correctly 85 per cent of the time, while our species succeeded
lust over half the time, "We didn't have the heart to tell these skilled
musicians that their performance was abysmal compared to a bunch of birds,"
says Weisman.
But there's good reason for these creatures' virtuoso performances: zebra
finches recognise members of their own species by the pitch range of their
songs. "And it you've ever heard birds in a dawn chorus, you know how hard
it is to distinguish one bird among the crowd," says Weisman.
Zebra finches can identity the absolute pitch of a mate's song up to 100
metres away, he says.
Songbirds and songwriters do have some things in common, though. Both birds
and humans with absolute pitch can often switch to relative pitch as well,
Weisman says. What's more, the birds hone their ear for pitch during youth,
when they listen for the calls of family members and neighbours. There are
some music lessons, Weisman notes, that really pay off.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|